Does team-based testing promote individual learning?

dc.contributor.advisorRobinson, Daniel H.en
dc.contributor.committeeMemberSchallert, Dianeen
dc.contributor.committeeMemberSvinicki, Marillaen
dc.contributor.committeeMemberBorich, Garyen
dc.contributor.committeeMemberMuir-Broaddus, Jacquelineen
dc.creatorWalker, Joshua Daviden
dc.date.accessioned2011-06-08T14:56:11Zen
dc.date.accessioned2011-06-08T14:56:30Zen
dc.date.accessioned2017-05-11T22:22:08Z
dc.date.available2011-06-08T14:56:11Zen
dc.date.available2011-06-08T14:56:30Zen
dc.date.available2017-05-11T22:22:08Z
dc.date.issued2011-05en
dc.date.submittedMay 2011en
dc.date.updated2011-06-08T14:56:30Zen
dc.descriptiontexten
dc.description.abstractTeam-based testing gives students a chance to earn additional points on individual unit tests by immediately re-taking the test as a team competing against other teams. This instructional approach has enjoyed widening implementation and impressive anecdotal support, but there remains a dearth of empirical studies evaluating its prescribed processes and promoted outcomes. Although the posited effectiveness and appeal of team-based testing seem consistent with the benefits of test-enhanced learning and collaborative learning in general, several limitations are readily apparent. Namely, the current format of the individual and team readiness assurance tests is expressly multiple-choice. Though there are some advantages of this type of question (e.g., ease of administering and grading), the long-term cognitive disadvantage relative to short-answer questions is well documented. Furthermore, it is not clear whether the proposed gain in learning through this format is attributable to the group effect -- be it social or cognitive, or simply to repeated exposure to the test items. Therefore, this study measured the effects of initial test question Format (short-answer vs. multiple-choice), Mode (individual vs. group), and Exposure (once vs. twice) on four delayed measures of learning: Old multiple-choice items (ones students had initially been tested over), Old short-answer items, New multiple-choice items, and New short-answer items. Two weeks after watching a video-recorded lecture, 208 college students took a thirty-item test comprising both the old and new items in multiple-choice and short-answer formats. Results revealed that 1) taking an initial test twice is better than once when the delayed test has old short-answer items or new multiple-choice items, 2) taking an initial short-answer test is better than multiple choice when the delayed test has either old multiple-choice, old short-answer, or new multiple-choice items, and 3) taking an initial team test is no different than taking an individual test when it comes to long-term learning. Particularly noteworthy from these results is how a) the effects of short-answer tests and taking tests twice are not present within Team conditions, and b) taking a multiple-choice test twice is as effective as taking a short-answer test once. Implications are discussed in light of learning theory and instructional practice.en
dc.description.departmentEducational Psychologyen
dc.format.mimetypeapplication/pdfen
dc.identifier.urihttp://hdl.handle.net/2152/ETD-UT-2011-05-3480en
dc.language.isoengen
dc.subjectTeam-based learningen
dc.subjectTesting effecten
dc.subjectCollaborative testingen
dc.subjectEducational testingen
dc.subjectTestingen
dc.subjectEducational tests and measurementsen
dc.titleDoes team-based testing promote individual learning?en
dc.type.genrethesisen

Files